Smaller Hippocampal Volume Predicts Pathologic Vulnerability to Psychological Trauma

  1. Research Service, Veterans Administration Medical Center, 718 Smyth Road, Manchester, NH 03104
  2. Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115
  3. Psychiatry Service, Veterans Administration Boston Healthcare System, Brockton, MA 02301
  4. Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, MA 02115
  5. Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02129

Originally published in Nature Neuroscience,2002, 5 (11), 1242-1247.
Reprinted at www.trauma-pages.com with permission of the authors. Note that this online version may have minor differences from the published version.
Correspondence should be addressed to: M W Gilbertson: Email: mark.gilbertson@med.va.gov

Acknowledgments: This work was supported by Department of Veterans Affairs Merit Review Grants (Gilbertson, Orr), USPHS Grant R01-MH54636 (Pitman), and USPHS Grant K02-MH01110 (Shenton). The authors would like to thank M. Macklin, K. Sheldon, S. Williston, L. Paulus, H. Croteau, and the VA Cooperative Studies VET Registry (M.E. Vitek, K. Bukowski, R. Havlicek, T. Colton, W.E. Nance, R.S. Paffenbarger, Jr., M.M. Weissman, R.R. Williams) for their assistance in this project. We gratefully acknowledge the participation of the veterans of the VET Registry and the non-Registry twin participants. Without their contribution this research would not have been possible.

Abstract

In animals, exposure to severe stress can damage the hippocampus. Recent human studies show smaller hippocampal volume in individuals with the stress-related psychiatric condition posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Does this represent the neurotoxic effect of trauma, or is smaller hippocampal volume a pre-existing condition that renders the brain more vulnerable to the development of pathological stress responses? In monozygotic twins discordant for trauma exposure, we found evidence that smaller hippocampi indeed constitute a risk factor for the development of stress-related psychopathology. Disorder severity in PTSD patients who were exposed to trauma was negatively correlated with the hippocampal volume of both the patients and the patients' trauma-unexposed identical co-twin. Furthermore, severe PTSD twin pairs-both the trauma-exposed and unexposed members-had significantly smaller hippocampi than non-PTSD pairs.

Animal research has provided compelling evidence that exposure to severe and chronic stress can damage the hippocampal formation,(1, 2) a region best known for its role in declarative memory (3, 4). Such studies point to a neurotoxic role for corticosteroids, elevated levels of which cause atrophy and/or cell death in hippocampal neurons. This has led to the proposal that a similar process may occur in humans, and thereby mediate specific stress-related disease processes. Of particular relevance is the psychiatric condition of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a constellation of disabling behavioral and emotional symptoms that occur in some individuals who experience severe psychological trauma such as combat, sexual abuse, or natural disaster. Indeed, several structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies report smaller hippocampal volume in patients diagnosed with chronic, unremitting forms of PTSD. (5-8) These results have generated intense interest regarding a potential pathogenesis for this disorder, and they raise the possibility that psychological trauma may in fact induce neurological damage in humans.

Controversy exists, however, over the nature and source of smaller hippocampal volume in PTSD. (9-12) The fundamental question at the heart of this controversy is whether volumetric differences represent the consequence of traumatic exposure, or a pre-existing trait that predisposes people to pathological stress reactions to a traumatic event. This latter formulation is consistent with the fact that only some individuals exposed to trauma go on to develop PTSD.(13,14) The National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study, (13) for example, has estimated the prevalence of PTSD in Vietnam combat veterans to be 30.6%. Furthermore, animal research shows that inherited variations in hippocampal size can influence behavioral outcomes in stress-mediated conditioning procedures (15-17) and can alter neuroendocrine responses to stress. (18) To date, there have been no human studies that directly address this important controversy.

In the present study, we used a 'case-control' design (Fig. 1) to examine samples of male monozygotic twin pairs in which one twin was a Vietnam combat veteran (exposed, Ex) and his identical co-twin had no combat exposure (unexposed, Ux). In some twin pairs, the combat-exposed brother developed chronic PTSD, whereas in other twin pairs the combat veteran never developed PTSD. Based on the diagnosis of the combat-exposed brother, we classified twin pairs into two groups: PTSD (P+) and non- (that is, never had) PTSD (P--). The P+ or P- designation always refers to the combat-related PTSD status of the exposed twin (no unexposed twin in this study had PTSD). Because monozygotic twins are genetically identical, any differences in hippocampal volume between brothers were interpreted as evidence for environmental effects, such as stress-induced neurotoxicity. Alternatively, any differences in hippocampal volume between the unexposed brothers of PTSD combat veterans (UxP+) versus the unexposed brothers of non-PTSD combat veterans (UxP-) were taken as evidence for a pre-existing trait. Amygdala and total brain volume served as control regions. Our results indicate that smaller hippocampal volume constitutes a pre-existing vulnerability factor for pathological response to stress.

[Figure 1]

Figure 1. Discordant monozygotic-twin paradigm for assessing origins of MRI differences in PTSD.

Sample coronal MRI images of right (red) and left (blue) hippocampi in a PTSD and a non-PTSD twin pair. Images represent four subject groups: (1) combat-exposed subjects who developed chronic PTSD (ExP+), (2) their combat unexposed co-twins with no PTSD themselves (UxP+), (3) combat-exposed subjects who never developed PTSD (ExPn), and (4) their unexposed co-twins also with no PTSD (UxPn). Contrast A provides a replication of previous work demonstrating smaller hippocampal volumes in combat veterans with versus without PTSD. Contrast B identifies the neurotoxicity effect, i.e., hippocampal reduction as environmentally acquired, by contrasting hippocampal volumes in combat-exposed PTSD veterans with their unexposed co-twins. Contrast C examines pre-existing vulnerability by contrasting hippocampal volumes in the two groups of combat-unexposed co-twins, whose combat-exposed brothers did versus did not develop PTSD. Model is tested by a Diagnosis (P+ vs Pn) x Exposure (Ex vs Ux) Analysis of Variance. Diagnosis refers to combat-exposed twin only. If hippocampal volume represents a vulnerability factor, the model predicts a significant Diagnosis main effect in the absence of a Diagnosis x Exposure interaction (i.e., PTSD combat-exposed veterans and their unexposed co-twins show same pattern). If hippocampal reduction results from neurotoxicity, the model predicts a significant Exposure main effect and/or Diagnosis x Exposure interaction.

Results

Brain volume correlations with post-trauma symptoms

Within-pair correlations for MRI brain volumes in the total sample were all highly significant (total brain volume: r = 0.90, P<0.0001; total hippocampus: r = 0.73, P<0.0001; total amygdala: r = 0.67, P<0.0001). Within the ExP+ subjects, there was a significant negative relationship (r = -0.64, P=0.006) between total hippocampal volume and PTSD symptom severity, as measured by the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) score (Fig. 2a). Thus, the hippocampal volume of exposed individuals was smaller in those with more severe PTSD symptoms. Importantly, there was also a significant negative correlation between hippocampal volume in UxP+ subjects and PTSD severity in their ExP+ brothers (r = -0.70, P=0.002; Fig. 2b), indicating that smaller hippocampal volume in identical co-twins who were not themselves exposed to combat was nonetheless related to more severe PTSD symptoms in their combat-exposed brothers. Adjusting for total brain volume, PTSD severity in the ExP+ twin remained significantly associated with both ExP+ (r = -0.54, P=0.03) and UxP+ (r = -0.61, P=0.01) hippocampal volumes. This indicates that the association between more PTSD symptoms in veterans and smaller hippocampal volumes in themselves and their co-twins were not explained by smaller overall brain volume. We did not find any significant correlations between PTSD severity and amygdala or total brain volume.

[Figure 2a]

Figure 2. Hippocampal volume correlations with post-trauma symptoms.

Scatter plots illustrate relationship of symptom severity in combat veterans with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) to: (a) their own hippocampal volumes, and (b) the hippocampal volumes of their monzygotic twin brothers who were not exposed to combat. Symptom severity represents the total score received on the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS).

[Figure 2b]

Combat severity (measured by a standardized combat exposure scale; see Methods) was not significantly related to total hippocampal volumes in any of the subject groups (ExP+, r = -0.32, P=0.21; all Ex combined, r = -0.08, P=0.64; UxP+, r = -0.11, P=0.66; all Ux combined, r = 0.01, P=0.97). Thus, the intensity level of stressful exposure in combat was not predictive of hippocampal volume in either exposed veterans or in their unexposed co-twins. A continuous measure of alcohol abuse history (Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test, MAST) was found to be related only to right hippocampal volume in ExP+ subjects (r = -0.51, P=0.04). However, this relationship was not evident in UxP+ subjects (r = 0.09, P=0.73). Furthermore, the relationship between total hippocampal volume in unexposed co-twins and PTSD symptom severity in their combat-exposed brothers remained significant after controlling for the effects of their own alcohol history (r = -0.70, P=0.004). Thus, whereas alcohol history had some relationship to hippocampal volume in combat veterans with PTSD, it was not related to hippocampal volume in their combat-unexposed brothers.

Brain volume differences in twin pair groups

Comparison of more severe PTSD cases (total CAPS>65; see Methods) with non-PTSD cases (Fig. 3) yielded a highly significant Diagnosis main effect for total hippocampal volume (Table 1).

TABLE 1. MRI Brain Volumes (ml): 2-Factor ANOVA for Redefined sample (PTSD Subjects with CAPS > 65)

PTSD (n=24) Non-PTSD (n=46) Two-factor ANOVA
Exposed
(n=12)
Unexposed
(n=12)
Exposed
(n=23)
Unexposed
(n=23)
Diagnosis
F 1,66 P
Exposure
F 1,66 P
Interaction F 1,66 P
Total brain volume 1221 (108) 1239 (125) 1258 (206) 1246 (112) 0.61 0.44 0.01 0.92 0.28 0.60
Total hippocampus 6.66 (0.83) 6.75 (0.90) 7.41 (0.93) 7.25 (0.69) 8.73 0.004 0.03 0.87 0.34 0.56
Right hippocampus 3.32 (0.59) 3.26 (0.39) 3.76 (0.54) 3.61 (0.48) 9.63 0.003 0.62 0.43 0.13 0.72
Left hippocampus 3.34 (0.46) 3.49 (0.57) 3.65 (0.50) 3.63 (0.45) 3.35 0.07 0.29 0.59 0.40 0.53
Total amygdala 4.65 (0.87) 4.53 (1.24) 4.45 (0.67) 4.61 (0.86) 0.07 0.80 0.01 0.92 0.40 0.53
Right amygdala 2.37 (0.52) 2.58 (0.64) 2.34 (0.39) 2.46 (0.48) 0.37 0.54 1.76 0.19 0.12 0.73
Left amygdala 2.27 (0.49) 1.95 (0.68) 2.11 (0.47) 2.15 (0.58) 0.02 0.90 1.06 0.31 1.76 0.19

Data given as mean (s.d.).

Follow-up t-tests:
Total Hippocampus: ExP+ vs ExP-, t(33)=2.32, p=.03; UxP+ vs UxP-, t(33)=1.83, p=.08; ExP+ vs UxP+, t(11)=0.38, p=.71; ExP- vs UxP-, t(22)=1.11, p=.28.
Right Hippocampus:ExP+ vs ExP-, t(33)=2.23, p=.03; UxP+ vs UxP-, t(33)=2.17, p=.04; ExP+ vs UxP+, t(11)=0.30, p=.77; ExP- vs UxP-, t(22)=1.42, p=.17.
Left Hippocampus: ExP+ vs ExP-, t(33)=1.74, p=.09; UxP+ vs UxP-, t(33)=0.85, p=.40; ExP+ vs UxP+, t(11)=1.31, p=.22; ExP- vs UxP-, t(22)=0.09, p=.93.

This result was unchanged after controlling for age (analysis of covariance (ANCOVA): F'[1,65]=8.63, P=0.005), combat severity in the Ex twin (F'[1,65]=6.72, P=0.01), and number of non-combat traumatic life events (F'[1,62]=4.67, P=0.03). Neither the main effect of exposure, nor the diagnosis x exposure interaction, was significant. Thus, hippocampal volumes were smaller in both the exposed and unexposed members of twin pairs in which the combat-exposed brother developed more severe PTSD, but there was no difference in hippocampal volume between brothers, regardless of combat or PTSD status. We did not find any significant effects for comparisons involving amygdala or total brain volumes.

[Figure 3]

Figure 3. Total hippocampal volumes for four subject groups.

Scatter plot illustrates absolute hippocampal volumes (ml) for combat-exposed individuals with and without PTSD, as well as their respective unexposed monozygotic co-twins. Data are only presented for PTSD twin pairs in which the combat-exposed twin had a CAPS score >65 (see text for explanation).

The main effect of diagnosis on hippocampal volume remained significant after removal of all subjects who reported childhood sexual or physical abuse (F[1,54]=4.77, P=0.03), and hippocampal volumes did not differ between abused and non-abused subjects in P+ twin pairs (t[32]=0.40, P=0.69) or in the sample as a whole (t[78]=0.95, P=0.34). Therefore, a previous history of childhood abuse was not relevant to the overall results.

The same pattern of statistical significance persisted with the addition of the excluded PTSD outlier (Methods) and when regional volumes were tested as a percentage of total brain volume. In fact, diagnosis remained a highly significant factor when controlling for overall brain volume (F'[1,65]=8.32, P=0.005) and amygdala volume (F'[1,65]=9.95, P=0.002) via ANCOVA. Thus, the observed hippocampal volume differences were specific relative to other brain regions examined. No significant main effects or interactions were observed in the two-factor ANOVA for hippocampal volumes in the full sample, which included PTSD subjects with total CAPS scores less than 65. Therefore, group differences emerged only when examining PTSD subjects with more severe symptoms and their co-twins.

Demographic and comorbidity features in twin pairs

ExP+ subjects had greater combat severity and PTSD symptom severity than ExP- subjects (Table 2). Age and education were similar between groups, although P+ pairs were slightly older. The highly significant interaction between diagnosis and exposure on the MAST indicates that combat veterans with PTSD had more severe alcohol abuse histories than the other three groups. No significant MAST score difference was found between UxP+ and UxP- subjects (CAPS>65 subsample comparison, t30=1.0, P=0.31), indicating that severity of alcohol abuse history did not explain the hippocampal differences in unexposed co-twins of PTSD versus non-PTSD combat veterans. For number of potentially traumatic lifetime events (non-combat related), PTSD combat veterans reported more of these than did non-PTSD combat veterans, and more events than their own unexposed co-twins. No significant difference was found in the reported number of traumatic lifetime events between UxP+ and UxP- subjects (CAPS>65 subsample comparison, t31=1.01, P=0.32), thus arguing against the relevance of lifetime non-combat trauma in the unexposed subjects as the explanation for the observed hippocampal volume differences in our sample. Within non-combat traumas, 29% of ExP+ subjects versus 13% of ExP- subjects (P=0.25, Fisher's exact test), and 24% of UxP+ vs. 9% of UxP- subjects (P=0.37) reported childhood sexual or physical abuse.

Table 2. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of PTSD and non-PTSD Twin Pairs.

  PTSD Non-PTSD Results of 2-factor ANOVAs
Exposed (n=17) Unexposed (n=17) Exposed (n=23) Unexposed (n=23) Diagnosis
F 1,76 P
Exposure
F 1,76 P
Interaction
F 1,76 P
Age (years) 53.1 (3.3) 53.1 (3.3) 51.8 (2.3) 51.8 (2.3) 3.8 .05 --- --- --- ---
Education (years) 13.5 (2.6) 14.3 (2.8) 14.7 (2.4) 14.7 (2.6) 1.8 .18 .45 .51 .55 .46
MAST* 20.2 (17.6) 6.8 (10.4) 2.4 (4.5) 2.5 (4.0) 22.8 <.001 8.4 .005 8.5 .005
Non-Combat Trauma
(number of events)
7.9 (2.6) 5.3 (3.8) 5.1 (4.0) 4.2 (3.0) 6.1 .02 4.9 .03 1.3 .26
Combat Severity 7.9 (1.9) --- 3.5 (2.6) --- 67.4 <.001 -- -- -- --
CAPS 72.2 (16.6) --- 6.2 (7.3) --- 577.7 <.001 -- -- -- --

* Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test. Data for 1 PTSD (both exposed and unexposed) and 2 non-PTSD (unexposed only) missing. Follow-up ttests: ExP+ vs ExP-, t[37]=4.63, p<.0001; UxP+ vs UxP-, t[35]=1.70, p=.09; ExP+ vs UxP+, t[15]=2.92, p=.01; ExP- vs UxP-, t[20]=.14, p=.89

† Lifetime number of non-combat potentially traumatic events. Data for 1 PTSD (exposed only) and 2 non-PTSD (unexposed only) missing. Follow-up ttests: ExP+ vs ExP-, t[37]=2.51, p=.02; UxP+ vs UxP-, t[36]=.96, p=.34; ExP+ vs UxP+, t[15]=2.51, p=.02; ExP- vs UxP, t[20]=.64, p=.53

‡ Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (total symptom score)

Lifetime comorbid alcohol abuse and dependence diagnoses were more frequent in ExP+ (82%) versus ExP- (43%) veterans (P=0.02), but there was no significant difference in rates between UxP+ (47%) and UxP- (30%) co-twins (P=0.34). The same pattern was found for group rates of lifetime other substance abuse or dependence disorders (53% for ExP+ versus 9% for ExP-, P=0.003; 6% for UxP+ versus 13% for UxP-, P=0.62) and lifetime Major Depressive Disorder (59% for ExP+ versus 13% for ExP-, P=0.005; 6% for UxP+ versus 4% for UxP-, P=0.99). Therefore, with regard to history of alcohol/substance abuse and major depression, combat veterans with PTSD showed significantly elevated rates compared with combat veterans without PTSD, whereas the unexposed co-twins of the former did not show significantly elevated rates compared to the unexposed co-twins of the latter.

Discussion

Consistent with previous reports (5-8), we found smaller hippocampal volume in trauma-exposed persons diagnosed with more severe, unremitting PTSD. The finding of a 10% difference in total hippocampal volume between individuals with versus without PTSD is in line with previously reported volumetric differences, as is the finding of predominant right hippocampal differences previously shown by male combat veterans. (5,19,20)

The key finding here concerns the identical twins of the higher-severity PTSD combat veterans, who were not themselves exposed to combat; they showed hippocampal volumes that were comparable to their combat-exposed brothers but significantly smaller than those of combat veterans without PTSD and their non-combat-exposed twins. These data indicate that smaller hippocampi in PTSD represent a pre-existing, familial vulnerability factor rather than the neurotoxic product of trauma exposure per se. Further support for this conclusion comes from the highly significant correlation that we found between the hippocampal volume of combat-unexposed co-twins and PTSD severity of their combat-exposed brothers. The high concordance of hippocampal volume within twin pairs, as well as the lack of a significant combat exposure effect or diagnosis x exposure interaction in our statistical model, provide clear evidence against the neurotoxicity hypothesis, as monozygotic co-twins provide the ideal biological control for detecting exposure-based differences. In light of the current findings, reference to hippocampal 'atrophy' in PTSD may be a misnomer.

We have also addressed the potential impact of confounding factors in the interpretation of hippocampal volume differences. In most non-twin studies, the presence of comorbid conditions precludes clear attribution of biological alterations to the psychiatric condition of PTSD alone. Most notably, major depression and alcohol abuse constitute comorbid conditions with high prevalence rates in PTSD (21) that could ostensibly also influence hippocampal volume. (22-25) Although previous studies have attempted to statistically control for such confounds, the controls can still be unsound if the covariate differs between groups. (26) If alcohol abuse and depression are secondary consequences of more severe PTSD, the effect of 'controlling for' these variables may simply be to remove variance associated with more severe PTSD. Our study design uniquely circumvented these difficulties. Specifically, the PTSD combat veterans with smaller hippocampi predictably showed higher rates of major depression and more severe alcohol histories, but their combat-unexposed twin brothers, who showed comparably small hippocampi, did not. These results effectively exclude these comorbid conditions as a source of hippocampal volume differences in PTSD. Moreover, the association of hippocampal volume with PTSD remained significant after adjusting for combat severity, which in this study was not significantly associated with hippocampal volume. This argues against the possibility that subjects with smaller hippocampi were more likely to be selected for high combat roles. These data do not counter the idea that more severe combat exposure results in more severe PTSD; rather, they suggest hippocampal volume to be a predictor of PTSD severity independent of combat severity.

A limitation of the present study relates to the fact that hippocampal differences, as revealed by structural MRI, may not apply to all populations of individuals diagnosed with PTSD. More specifically, pre-existing decreased hippocampal volume may only be related to severe and unremitting forms of posttraumatic stress responses. All studies to date that have found smaller hippocampal volume in PTSD, including the findings reported here, have involved individuals with chronic, unremitting forms of the disorder; that is, intense symptoms which persist for years, and in many cases, decades. In fact, group differences in hippocampal volume only emerged in our sample when we examined PTSD individuals with a CAPS symptom severity score above 65. Failures to replicate findings of reduced hippocampal volume in PTSD have typically been reported in studies that involved subjects with PTSD of lower severity and/or shorter duration. (27-29) Over 40% of those diagnosed with PTSD show remission within the first year after traumatic exposure, (30, 31) with a continued, more gradual remission rate for approximately six years. (14) Such individuals may clearly differ from those who develop long-standing, unremitting posttraumatic symptoms.

Hippocampal morphology and function have been implicated in conditioning and extinction of fear responses in animals, and may be involved in the contextual processing of fear. (32, 33) Rodents with hippocampal lesions show stronger conditioned fear, as evidenced by more rapid acquisition of an avoidance response to an auditory cue paired with shock, as well as more fear behavior following acquisition, than do non-lesioned animals. (34, 35) Similar alterations in fear-mediated performance have also been shown in mice with genetically smaller hippocampi. (15-17) Smaller hippocampal volume may also predispose an animal to diminished neuroendocrine regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, as has been shown in monkeys who inherit smaller hippocampi and respond to stressful rearing conditions with larger cortisol elevations. (18) As a vulnerability factor for PTSD, smaller hippocampal volume might therefore predispose individuals to acquire stronger and/or more persistent conditioned emotional responses, or stronger hormonal stress responses, when exposed to a traumatic event. (36)

Heredity is the most likely explanation for the origin of the smaller hippocampi observed in PTSD combat veterans and their twins in this study. (18, 37-40) In the absence of dizygotic twin subjects, however, the effects of heredity could not be separated from those of shared environment. We did find a non-significant trend for P+ pairs to share higher rates of childhood abuse, but this did not account for the observed hippocampal differences. Additionally, unexposed co-twins of PTSD veterans did not share with their brothers a general increase in lifetime number of reported non-combat trauma or stressor incidents, further diminishing the relevance of shared environment. Moreover, any stress-based interpretation of the smaller hippocampal volume observed in the combat-unexposed co-twins of the PTSD veterans would need to explain why the extra stress of military combat and consequent PTSD did not exert any further reduction in hippocampal volume in the PTSD veterans. Indeed, the finding that individuals who were exposed to combat but did not develop PTSD, had larger hippocampi than individuals who were not exposed to combat but were merely the brothers of combat veterans with PTSD argues strongly against a stress-neurotoxic interpretation of the hippocampal diminution. Nevertheless, further research that includes dizygotic twin pairs is needed to tease apart the contributions of genetics and shared environment to smaller hippocampi in PTSD.

Methods

Subjects. All subjects were recruited with the assistance of the Vietnam Era Twin (VET) Registry, which determined zygosity (41) and combat status (via military records) of each twin pair. Combat severity was assessed using a standard 18-item combat exposure measure. (42) Complete descriptions of the development and characteristics of the VET Registry have appeared elsewhere. (43, 44) All subjects had previously participated in a larger twin study of PTSD at our laboratory; a full description of the recruitment strategy appears elsewhere. (45) Twin pairs in which the combat-exposed brother never had PTSD were recruited directly from the Registry. Owing to competing demands on their time, twin pairs in which the combat-exposed brother had PTSD were unavailable from the Registry. Hence, these pairs were recruited by a mass mailing to Vietnam veterans who had a service-connected disability for PTSD; in fact, these pairs better approximated the PTSD veterans from our previous (non-twin) study of hippocampal volume (6) with regard to clinical severity. Although concern might be raised regarding the different sources for PTSD versus non-PTSD twin pairs, the significant correlations observed between PTSD severity and hippocampal volume within the PTSD twin pairs alone mitigates the likelihood that recruitment differences can explain the observed group differences.

The protocol was approved by the Veterans Administration (Manchester, New Hampshire) Institutional Review Board and Human Subjects Subcommittee, and all subjects gave informed written consent prior to participation. PTSD diagnostic statuses of combat-exposed twins and their overall PTSD symptom severity were determined by an experienced doctoral-level psychologist using the CAPS (46). All subjects completed a stressful life events checklist (available upon request) that was designed to quantify the lifetime number of non-combat events that potentially met DSM-IV PTSD A (stressor) criteria. Subjects were also interviewed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (47) to determine the presence of other Axis I mental disorders. The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST) (48) was used as a measure of lifetime alcohol abuse. Subjects were excluded if they met DSM-IV criteria for a psychotic or bipolar disorder, or non-combat-related PTSD. Due to the high comorbidity of major depression and substance abuse disorders with PTSD, these disorders did not represent exclusion criteria. The final sample comprised 17 PTSD twin pairs and 23 non-PTSD twin pairs.

MRI Image Acquisition and Volumetric Analyses. MRI scanning was performed at the Brigham and Women's Hospital in Boston with a 1.5 tesla General Electric Signa System (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, Wisconsin) using previously described techniques (see below). (6, 49) Whole-brain volume was calculated with automated multistep algorithms described in detail elsewhere. (49) Hippocampus and amygdala were outlined manually on a Sun Microsystems workstation by a rater blind to diagnostic information and twin status, using an established procedure for volumetric determination. (6, 49) Regional volumes were also expressed as percentages of whole brain volume and reanalyzed to confirm the results of absolute volume analyses. A second blind rater performed volumetric analyses of the hippocampus and amygdala on five random twin pair cases. Reliability assessment of the two raters resulted in the following intraclass correlation coefficients: right hippocampus = 0.96; left hippocampus = 0.92; right amygdala = 0.98, left amygdala = 0.97.

Sagittal localizer images were obtained first, followed by double-echo spin-echo axial slices of the brain (108 contiguous slices; 54 for each level) with repetition time (TR)=3000msec, echo time (TE)=30 and 80msec, slice thickness=3mm, field of view=24cm, acquisition matrix=256x256 (192 phase-encoding steps, with zero filling), voxel dimensions=0.9375 x 0.9375 x 3mm. For the amygdala-hippocampal complex, 3-D Fourier transform spoiled gradient-recalled acquisition (3DFT) in steady state was reformatted into 124 contiguous 1.5mm coronal slices: TE=5msec with one repetition, TR=35msec, nutation angle=45 degrees, field of view=24cm, acquisition matrix=256 by 256 (192 phase-encoding steps) by 124, voxel dimensions = 0.9375 x 0.9375 x 1.5mm.

The following subject exclusions occurred in the course of MRI image acquisition. Two subjects were excluded on the basis of a significant abnormality discovered upon MRI scanning which could impact volumetric measurements, e.g., tumor; the affected individuals were provided with this information and arrangements made for appropriate clinical transfer of MRI reports. Three subjects were unable to complete MRI due to claustrophobic reactions, and one subject due to a cardiac condition. Two MRI scans were not segmented for volumetric measurement due to excessive movement artifact. Data from both members of a twin pair were excluded from the analyses if either member was excluded.

Statistical Analyses. All statistical analyses were performed using two-tailed tests. Hippocampus and amygdala volumes were found to be normally distributed in the overall sample based upon a Shapiro-Wilk goodness-of-fit statistic with null hypothesis rejection set at P<0.15. One case (ExP+) was removed from the analyses because of extreme outlier status on three characteristics of hippocampal morphology (right hippocampus >2.5 s.d. above the full sample mean, right hippocampus 1.9 s.d larger than left hippocampus, and right hippocampus 1.7 s.d. larger than co-twin). No other subject showed similarly extreme values. This outlier and his twin were removed to avoid obscuring any potentially important correlations between variables. Nevertheless, critical group comparisons were performed both with and without this twin pair, to ensure that the observed differences were not due solely to their exclusion.

Pearson correlations between neuroanatomical volumes on the one hand, and clinical/psychometric characteristics on the other, were performed within PTSD twin pairs only, because of the low, restricted range of PTSD symptom severity in the non-PTSD twin pairs (75% of sample had total CAPS score <10). Group differences were tested by two-factor ANOVA with one between-pair factor, diagnosis (P+ versus P- in the Ex twin), in the exposed twin, and one within-pair factor, combat Exposure (Ex versus Ux). Because a CAPS score of 65 has been established as an optimal cutoff for creating an unambiguous PTSD group, (50) we conducted ANOVA tests both in the complete sample and in a redefined sub-sample that included only those ExP+ subjects who met this criterion for symptom severity and their twins.

References

1. Sapolsky, R. M., Uno, H., Rebert, C. S. & Finch, C. E. Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged glucocorticoid exposure in primates. J. Neurosci. 10, 2897-2902 (1990).

2. McEwen, B. S. in The Cognitive Neurosciences (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 1117-1135 (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995).

3. Squire, L. R. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol. Rev. 99, 195-231 (1992).

4. Zola-Morgan, S. & Squire, L. R. Neuroanatomy of memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 547-563 (1993).

5. Bremner, J. D. et al. MRI-based measurements of hippocampal volume in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 973-978 (1995).

6. Gurvits, T. V. et al. Magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal volume in chronic, combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 40, 1091-1099 (1996).

7. Bremner, J. D. et al. Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse: a preliminary report. Biol. Psychiatry 41, 23-32 (1997).

8. Stein, M. B., Koverola, C., Hanna, C., Torchia, M. G. & McClarty, B. Hippocampal volume in women victimized by childhood sexual abuse. Psychol. Med. 27, 951-959 (1997).

9. Pitman, R. K. Hippocampal diminution in PTSD: more (or less?) than meets the eye. Hippocampus 11, 73-74 (2001).

10. Bremner, J. D. Hypotheses and controversies related to effects of stress on the hippocampus: an argument for stress-induced damage to the hippocampus in patients with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Hippocampus 11, 75-81 (2001).

11. McEwen, B. S. Commentary on PTSD discussion. Hippocampus 11, 82-84 (2001).

12. Yehuda, R. Are glucocortoids responsible for putative hippocampal damage in PTSD? How and when to decide. Hippocampus 11, 85-89 (2001).

13. Kulka, R. A. et al. Trauma and the Vietnam War Generation: Report of Findings from the National VietnamVeterans Readjustment Study (Brunner/Mazel, New York, 1990).

14. Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Huges, M. & Nelson, C. B. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52, 1048-1060 (1995).

15. Crusio, W. E., Schwegler, H. & van Abeelen, J. H. F. Behavioral responses to novelty and structural variation of the hippocampus in mice. II. Multivariate genetic analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 32, 81-88 (1989).

16. Wimer, C. C., Wimer, R. E. & Roderick, T. H. Some behavioral differences associated with relative size of hippocampus in the mouse. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 76, 57-65 (1971).

17. Schwegler, H. & Lipp, H. P. Hereditary covariations of neuronal circuitry and behavior: correlations between the proportions of hippocampal synaptic fields in the regio inferior and two-way avoidance in mice and rats. Behav. Brain Res. 7, 1-38 (1983).

18. Lyons, D. M., Yang, C., Sawyer-Glover, A. M., Moseley, M. E. & Schatzberg, A. F. Early life stress and inherited variation in monkey hippocampal volumes. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58, 1145-1151 (2001).

19. Freeman, T. W., Cardwell, D., Karson, C. N. & Komoroski, R. A. In vivo proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the medial temporal lobes of subjects with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Magn. Reson. Med. 40, 66-71 (1998).

20. Schuff, N. et al. Reduced hippocampal volume and n-acetyl aspartate in posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.; 821, 516-520 (1997).

21. Keane, T. M. & Kaloupek, D. G. Comorbid psychiatric disorders in PTSD: implications for research. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 821, 24-34 (1997).

22. Bremner, J. et al. Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 115-127 (2000).

23. Laakso, M.P. et al. A volumetric MRI study of the hippocampus in type 1 and 2 alcoholism. Behav. Brain Res. 109, 177-186 (2000).

24. Sheline, Y., Sanghavi, M., Mintin, M. & Gado, M. Depression duration but not age predicts hippocampal volume loss in medical healthy women with recurrent major depression. J. Neurosci. 19, 5034-5041 (1999).

25. De Bellis, M. D. et al. Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 737-744 (2000).

26. Miller, G. A. & Chapman, J. P. Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 110, 40-48 (2001).

27. De Bellis, M. D. et al. Developmental traumatology part II : brain development. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 1271-1284 (1999).

28. Schuff, N. et al. Decreased hippocampal n-acetylaspartate in the absence of atrophy in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 952-959 (2001).

29. Bonne, O. et al. Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1248-1251 (2001).

30. Breslau, N. & Davis, G. C. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in an urban population of young adults: risk factors for chronicity. Am. J. Psychiatry 149, 671-675 (1992).

31. Mellman, T. A., Randolph, C. A., Brawman-Mintzer, O., Flores, L. P. & Milanes, F. J. Phenomenology and course of psychiatric disorders associated with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 149, 1568-1574 (1992).

32. Selden, N. R. W., Everitt B. J., Jarrard, L. E. & Robbins, T. W. Complementary roles for amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 42, 335-350 (1991).

33. Phillips, R. G. & LeDoux, J. E. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 274-285 (1992).

34. Falls, W. A. & Davis, M. in Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Stress: from Normal Adaptation to Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (eds. Friedman, D. J., Charney, D. S. & Deutch, A.Y.) 177-202 (Lippincott-Raven, Philadelphia, 1995).

35. Antelman, S. M. & Brown, T. S. Hippocampal lesions and shuttlebox avoidance behavior: a fear hypothesis. Physiol. Behav. 9, 15-20 (1972).

36. Orr, S. P. et al. De novo conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 290-298 (2000).

37. Wimer, R. E. & Wimer, C. C. A biometrical-genetic analysis of granule cell number in the area dentata of house mice. Brain Res. 254, 129-140 (1981).

38. Beck, K. D., Powell-Braxton, L., Widmer, H. R., Vlaverde, J. & Hefti, F. Igfl gene disruption results in reduced brain size, CNS hypomyelination, and loss of hippocampal granule and striatal parvalbumin-containing neurons. Neuron 14, 717-730 (1995).

39. Pennington, B. F. et al. A twin MRI study of size variations in the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 223-232 (2000).

40. Thompson, P. M. et al. Genetic influences on brain structure. Nat. Neurosci. 4,1253-1258 (2001).

41. Eisen, S., Neuman, R., Goldberg, J., Rice, J. & True, W. Determining zygosity in the Vietnam Era Twin Registry: an approach using questionnaires. Clin. Genet. 35, 423-432 (1989).

42. Janes, G. R., Goldberg, J., Eisen, S. A. & True, W. R. Reliability and validity of a combat exposure index for Vietnam era veterans. J. Clin. Psychol. 47, 80-86 (1991).

43. Eisen, S. A., True, W. R., Goldberg, J., Henderson, W. & Robinette, C. D. The Vietnam Era Twin (VET) Registry: method of construction. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae (Roma) 36, 61-66 (1987).

44. Henderson, W. G. et al. The Vietnam Era Twin Registry: a resource for medical research. Public Health Rep. 105, 368-373 (1990).

45. Orr, S. P. et al. Physiologic responses to sudden, loud tones in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: association with PTSD. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, in press.

46. Blake, D. D. et al. The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. J. Trauma. Stress 8, 75-90 (1995).

47. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M. & Williams, J. B.W. Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders, Version 2.0. (Biometrics Research Department, New York, 1994).

48. Selzer, M. L. The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: the quest for a new diagnostic instrument. Am. J. Psychiatry 127, 1653-1658 (1971).

49. Shenton, M. A. et al. Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imagery study. N. Engl. J. Med. 327, 604-612 (1992).

50. Weathers, F. W., Ruscio, A. M. & Keane, T. M. Psychometric properties of nine scoring rules for the Clinican-Administered Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Scale. Psychol. Assess. 11, 124-133 (1999).